Bronze-level articlePlacebo effect

From RationalWiki
Jump to: navigation, search
Tell me about your mother

Psychology

Icon psychology.svg
Let us examine...
Random examples
No one doubts—certainly not I—that the mind exercises a powerful influence over the body. From the beginning of time, the sorcerer, the interpreter of dreams, the fortune-teller, the charlatan, the quack, the wild medicine-man, the educated physician, the mesmerist, and the hypnotist have made use of the client's imagination to help them in their work. They have all recognized the potency and availability of that force. Physicians cure many patients with a bread pill; they know that where the disease is only a fancy, the patient's confidence in the doctor will make the bread pill effective.
Mark Twain, Christian Science, 1907.

The placebo effect is a psychosomatic phenomenon in which symptoms of a disease or condition lessen — or even appear to be cured completely — from the patient being merely exposed to a treatment. Believing that their condition will be improved, they will begin to feel better and perhaps identifiable symptoms may disappear, irrespective of whether the treatment has any chemical or pharmacological effect. Because of this, controlling for the placebo effect is an essential part of medicine. For example, sugar pills or saline solutions, which have no pharmacological properties, are commonly prescribed as placebo treatments to control for the effect the simple act of intervening in a patient's illness may have.

Most alternative medicine is suggested to work through this method[1] — mostly because any treatment that doesn't work through this method is labelled "medicine". Under placebo control, alternative medicines do not perform significantly better.

Contents

[edit] Causes

[edit] Statistical vs individual experience

The placebo effect is quite a complex phenomenon. It is influenced by a multitude of things, including positive thought, reduced stress, the intensity or "drama" of the medical intervention, a patient's expectations of what the treatment can do, and the wider cultural meaning of medical treatment. A particularly interesting observation is how much these factors can be influenced subconsciously; a doctor merely telling someone a treatment will work may not produce such a dramatic effect if the doctor doesn't actually believe it too. Because of the complexity of issues surrounding what causes a sick patient to get "better", it's difficult to say if an individual was cured by a placebo effect, real treatment, regression to the mean or a complicated combination of all three. So, the placebo effect is best described as a statistical phenomenon, where prognosis improves for a certain percentage of patients simply exposed to a treatment of some kind.

However, as the placebo effect is powered by belief in a treatment and exposure to it it can manifest in individuals to a certain extent. A homeopathic prescription of water containing a minute dilution of powdered oyster shell may bring some improvement to a patient who has total faith in the homeopathic method. A patient who is skeptical of homeopathy may experience a reduced effect or no effect at all, as demonstrated by James Randi when he regularly consumes massive overdoses of homeopathic sleeping pills on stage. If these two hypothetical patients begin to feel better anyway, the former would be likely to attribute it to the treatment while the latter would be likely to dismiss it as just feeling better anyway. This leads to selective reporting, and is why anecdotal evidence shouldn't be trusted over statistical data.

[edit] Expectation effects

Expectations of what drugs and medical treatments can, and do, change over time. Often the efficacy of real drugs decreases when a new version is released, as people expect the new one to be better. New treatments, which are perceived to be more effective because they exploit new technologies induce a placebo effect in addition to their real effect and, sometimes, this can lead to an over-estimate of their real efficacy over an existing treatment. This is why in controlled studies of new treatments, they are compared not with inactive placebos but the best effective treatment available (and not least because it is unethical to use placebopathy where a working treatment is known) and the experience between patients is minimised so that they genuinely cannot tell the difference between treatment A and treatment B.

It is an observed phenomenon that placebo drugs distributed in flashier packaging tend to produce a stronger reaction than their plain packaging counterparts.[2] There is also evidence that the placebo effect can work even if the person knows that it is a placebo, as observed in a study of patients with irritable bowel syndrome.[3] However, the statement issued to patients read "placebo pills made of an inert substance, like sugar pills, that have been shown in clinical studies to produce significant improvement in IBS symptoms through mind-body self-healing processes" - which still creates an expectation of treatment despite being told there was no chemical reason that such treatment would help.

In short, the placebo effect works if you expect the placebo effect to work.

[edit] Ethical considerations

There are a number of ethical issues surrounding the wilful use of placebos as a treatment - known as placebopathy. Many casual complaints can be dealt with by inactive placebo treatments (involving a sugar pill and a reassuring visit to a doctor) and this would save a significant amount of time, money and lowers the risk of drug dependency and overdose. Yet, despite these potential benefits to willingly issuing placebos, it is not considered ethical in modern medicine. Firstly, placebopathy involves medical professions deceiving their patients, which is considered bad ethical practice, and secondly there is a risk in delaying the treatment of a real condition due to a doctor mistrusting or underestimating their patient's symptoms. If a patient complaining of a headache is given a sugar pill to placate it, and it turns out to be a serious condition that requires treatment then at the very least this would be a serious law suit waiting to happen.

Conversely, some medications that have been prescribed have later turned out to be no more effective than inactive placebos, yet can be an effective treatment for psychosomatic conditions or short-term illnesses. Because any treatment tends to produce a better effect than no treatment at all (hence the need for placebo control in trials, see below) it could be considered unethical to stop this practice of effective placebopathy.

[edit] Use in medical trials

Oscillococcinum, a well-known homeopathic remedy that "works" by the placebo effect.

Because the placebo effect is so powerful, efficacy claims for medical treatments must be tested in an experiment that controls for this effect. This is usually achieved by running a double-blind test, where some patients receive the treatment being examined, and others get a placebo. Neither the patients or the administrators of the treatment know who is in which group. In order for the treatment to be proved useful, it must produce results that are better than the placebo at a statistically significant level. In practice the "placebo" is usually an existing treatment, as being better than an placebo isn't enough - a new treatment has to be better than the existing one! For example, in trials of keyhole surgery over conventional open heart surgery, elaborate schemes were undertaken to make patients all feel as if they had the same treatment - but all were treated for their condition, none received a full "fake" treatment that did nothing. Similarly, new pills would be compared with the existing treatments they aim to replace, not necessarily sugar pills or more "powerful" (but more restricted) drugs.

Placebo treatments can often be quite difficult to design, but are still possible. As mentioned above, placebo surgeries have been performed using quite elaborate theatre, and often the precise wording that a doctor must use to describe a treatment to a patient is strictly controlled. Such treatments can be used to test not just efficacy, but the explanations offered by alternative medicine practitioners. A placebo treatment to test acupuncture, for example, could be designed to test claims about meridian lines would involve sticking the needles into a patient but in more randomised areas rather than the specific places the technique claims are "special". A second placebo treatment could test the effect of needles by changing the depth which they penetrate into the skin - acupuncture needles often go deeper than you think, while placebo needles penetrate only the top layer of the skin. Thirdly the effect of a visit to a practitioner can be controlled for by using entirely "sham" needles which prick the skin, causing indistinguishable pain sensations, but otherwise don't penetrate or cause any lasting effect. These three methods combine to test three separate areas whereby an alternative medicine can work.

Quacks who push woo treatments rely on the placebo effect when arguing that their "cures" have an effect. This is exactly why medical tests need to pit a drug against a placebo: anything will usually perform better than nothing. Often, proponents of alternative medicine will reject placebo controlled experiments for any number of excuses. For example, many homopathic practitioners assert that treatments must be assigned individually and so are unsuited to the format of a randomised controlled trial. Yet, such things can be overcome by simply exposing the patient to everything about the treatment, including this individual treatment "design", and simply swapping the medication at the last moment, unknown to both the patient and practitioner. This sort of assertion is common in alternative medicine, but is obviously at odds with the mass-produced remedies that are sold to the public. Such trials have been done (very rarely does the alternative medicine world foot the bill for these), and they invariably come up negative.

[edit] The strangeness of the placebo effect

The power of the placebo effect produces many strange and interesting quirks. These indicate that the placebo effect may well be more than mere "mind over matter", and stem from what is known as a complex intervention. This is where any given treatment consists of not just the medicine issued, but the bedside manner of the doctor, how the treatment is described and sold, and how society and environment shape our expectations of what a treatment can and cannot do. Many of these examples and their ramifications are discussed in slightly more detail in Ben Goldacre's book Bad Science.

  • The effect has been observed in animals and babies. However, in practice this requires conditioning first, which is analogous to just telling someone a treatment will work when you can't communicate more directly.[4][5] This is contrary to claims by homeopaths who claim that their remedies work in babies and animals and so cannot be due to the placebo effect. (Homeopathic teething tablets can make babies stop crying, but because they're made of lactose and sucrose (milk and sugar), not because of the homeopathic water.)
  • Placebos that are "more expensive" work better,[6] as do ones that come in fancier packaging.[7]
  • The effect shows a dose-response relationship like a real medical intervention; two pills are more effective than one.
  • More dramatic placebos are more effective. Injections work better than pills. A full consultation session with a doctor prior to a placebo is more effective than just giving a patient a pill dismissively.
  • The placebo effect may possibly have the ability to overpower real pharmacological effects. People who were given very strong drugs to induce nausea didn't develop symptoms as they were told the treatment would actually relieve nausea.[8]
  • There are reports from war zones where saline solutions were used due to shortages of morphine, and proved just as effective in battlefield surgery.[citation needed]
  • In one trial, the placebo effect was induced even though the individuals were told that it was a placebo that contains no medicine; although they were told that it was shown to be of help to people with such a condition.[3]
  • As an indication of the wider cultural meaning and subconscious activity of the effect, it is observed that the colour of a sugar pill also affects the placebo response. Certain colours boost the efficacy of placebos for different illnesses, and can even correlate with gender.[9]
  • The placebo effect may actually be getting stronger over time, as expectations about the power of medicine change,[10] but at the same time it can cause drugs that are old to become weaker as the expectation is that new and novel treatments must be better.
  • The placebo effect may be transferable; a patient being around someone who believes that the treatment will work can cause the patient to do better - even if that person is a doctor.[11] This is the reason for double-blinding in drug trials. It may also mean that prayer can affect patients if they know they're being prayed for; rigorous studies of prayer therefore make sure that there is no contact between prayers and patients.
  • While not related to medicine in and of itself, Tummo meditation is one example of the power of the placebo effect. People practicing it (the most famous being Wim Hof) can keep their body temperature warm even in extremely cold temperatures just by using their mind.

[edit] Nocebo effect

The placebo effect has a somewhat less well-known, slightly evil cousin; the nocebo effect.[12][13] This occurs when an individual's expectation of negative effects generate those effects, much like a hypochondriac developing symptoms because he or she is actively watching for something to go wrong with their health. This is particularly important when looking at side-effects of drugs, as a placebo pill can generate the negative side-effects associated with drugs, and in some cases, specific side-effects can be increased by mentioning them prominently or adding an extra layer of suggestion, such as complex consent forms.[14]

For ethical reasons the nocebo effect has not been well studied directly, but it is probably behind the witch doctor's ability to cause illness in those people he curses. It has also been used to explain unusual phenomenon like SUNDS (Sudden Unexpected Nocturnal Death Syndrome) in immigrants from the Hmong people of Laos; belief in angry supernatural forces combined with the phenomenon of sleep paralysis to convince sufferers that they were under attack, leading to extremely high levels of stress which could trigger dormant heart defects.[15]

[edit] False placebo

A false placebo is a non-placebo factor that appears to cause an improvement of a disease. False placebos differ from actual placebos in that they occur regardless of whether the patient is treated or not. False placebos are the lesser-known things controlled for in the placebo arms of clinical trials. Some examples of false placebos include misdiagnosis, regression to the mean, and spontaneous regression.

[edit] See also

[edit] Footnotes

  1. Scientific American - Placebo Effect: A Cure in the Mind
  2. Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches, A Branthwaite and P Cooper
  3. 3.0 3.1 Kaptchuk TJ, Friedlander E, Kelley JM, Sanchez MN, Kokkotou E, et al. (2010) Placebos without Deception: A Randomized Controlled Trial in Irritable Bowel Syndrome. PLoS ONE 5(12)
  4. Science Based Medicine - Is There a Placebo Effect for Animals?
  5. Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression, R Ader and N Cohen, Psychosomatic Medicine, Vol 37, Issue 4 333-340
  6. Expensive Placebo Works Better Than Cheap One
  7. Analgesic effects of branding in treatment of headaches, A Branthwaite and P Cooper
  8. Wolf S. Effects of suggestions and conditioning on the action of chemical agents in human subjects - the pharmacology of placebos. J Clin Invest 1950;29:100-9. PMCID PMC439730
  9. Lucchelli, Cattaneo, Zattoni, Effect of capsule colour and order of administration of hypnotic treatments, European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 2004
  10. Placebos Are Getting More Effective. Drugmakers Are Desperate to Know Why, Wired Magazine.
  11. a) Gracely R.H,; Dubner, R.; Deeter, W.R.; Wolksee, P.J., Clinclicians' expectations influence placebo analgesia. Lancet, 1, 43 8419 (1985) b) via Ben Goldacre, Bad Science, p.75.
  12. nocebo effect
  13. New Scientist - when mind attacks body
  14. Bad Science - All bow before the mighty power of the nocebo effect
  15. Adler, Shelley R. Sleep Paralysis. Rutgers: 2011.
Personal tools
Namespaces

Variants
Actions
Navigation
Community
Toolbox
support